If you're seeing this message, it means we're having trouble loading external resources on our website.

If you're behind a web filter, please make sure that the domains *.kastatic.org and *.kasandbox.org are unblocked.

Main content

Heritability

Heritability measures how much of a trait's variation in a population can be linked to genetic differences. It's not about how much a trait is genetic, but how much genetic differences account for variations. This concept is crucial in understanding intelligence, behavior, and other traits. Heritability depends on the population and environment studied. Created by Ryan Scott Patton.

Want to join the conversation?

  • female robot amelia style avatar for user peppered
    if we took four genetically identical children and raised them in barrels like the Mark Twain example in the video, would they have no differences in IQ?
    (14 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • leafers tree style avatar for user brenasuarus
      Correct. Both their genetics and environment would be exactly the same. This means that their IQ would be 100% heritable, thus 100% dependent on their genome. If you want to get more complicated, their epigenomes would not necessarily be the same however (we see differences in epigenomes just from having different placentas or amniotic sacs in utero), so that could confound the example.
      (13 votes)
  • starky sapling style avatar for user Toby Johnson
    Why isn't h^2 for all homozygous twin studies 0 then? Because they share 100% genes so surely all the difference is attributable to the environment?
    (2 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • hopper jumping style avatar for user Eric
    When the speaker says "h two" does he mean "h squared"?
    h = the % variation of traits due to genes
    h^2 = (the % variation of traits due to genes)^2
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • piceratops tree style avatar for user nikhiljaiswal95
    So taking a big stab at this:

    Is h^2 (the amount of variance attributed to genetic material), similar to r^2, the statistical variable identifying how variation can be explained by a graph?

    Also, can someone explain r^2 for me?

    Thank you so much for your help!
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • piceratops tree style avatar for user nikhiljaiswal95
    Can someone also clarify what Ryan means from -?
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • blobby green style avatar for user francesmorales819
    to be sure that I have this right, are you saying, for example, that the difference in IQ between two people is 50% a result of their genetic differences (variation)?
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • blobby green style avatar for user thoonset43
    CAN ANYONE PLEASE HELP ME OUT

    At 1.00, he said IQ is 50 percent heritable. This does not mean IQ is 50 percent genetic; difference in intelligence is 50 percent attributable to variation in genes.

    When talking about variation, we are talking about the differences between individuals in a given study. So, the similarities between individuals are not accounted. For this reason, we cannot say that 50 percent of the IQ in an individual is caused by differences in genes. What we can say is that 50 percent of the variation in IQ between individuals can be explained by differences in genes.

    As a result, heritability studies can only tell us the extent to which differences in genes are responsible for variation in a trait between individuals; and not how much each genes contribute to a given trait individually.

    Is this line of reasoning correct or incorrect? If incorrect, can you please explain why.
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • female robot grace style avatar for user Amelia Milburn
    If you had four quadruplets with exactly the same genome and raised them in controlled environments so that their environment was exactly the same, would they be identical?
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • leaf green style avatar for user Abdullah
      Yes, if they all share the same genome, then they will be identical quadruplets. And since they share the same environment, there would be less variation in terms of different genes being expressed.

      Also, interestingly, quadruplets can vary. You can either have all 4 babies be different from each other (i.e 4 eggs were each fertilized by a different sperm). You can have all 4 babies be identical (i.e. one egg was fertilized by a sperm, and then splits three times to form 4 embryos). And then a few other combination such as 3 identical triplets + 1 more (i.e 2 eggs were fertilized, but one of the eggs split twice to form the identical triplets). Or identical twins + 2 others (i.e 3 eggs were fertilized, and one egg was split once to get the identical twins).
      (1 vote)
  • aqualine seed style avatar for user KaraBean2005
    Okay I understand this but , Fertal twins raised together and are close v.s. Identical twins raised apart but grow to be best friends, we see a strong commute here but they grow up not seeing eachother through the eyes that they should of , What would happen if they grow up never learning they were twins? And the children they have don't know that their close cousins.
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • duskpin ultimate style avatar for user Archi
      That would be a very rare case. And, I believe you meant to type in "Fraternal or Fertile " instead of "Fertal twins". I don't really see the problem if twins grow/grew up in different places.Perhaps if you rephrased your question, I could give you a better answer. Though right now I'm confused on what you mean.
      (0 votes)
  • blobby green style avatar for user Jiamin Cn
    For quadruplets raised in the different environments, I understand that based on the equation, heritability would increase. However, conceptually I do not get it. Conceptually, I think of heritability as the likelihood to inherit a specific trait. Is this a wrong way to look at it conceptually?
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user

Video transcript

OK. So I ended the last video, the one on twin and adoption studies, with some pretty specific language. I said that through those studies, psychologists could estimate the extent to which variation of traits, or attributes, for any given population-- the extent to which that variation can be attributed to differences in genes. And I used that language, that specific language because I was actually defining the term "heritability." So a textbook might define the term heritability as the proportion of variation-- so a percentage, because a proportion is just a percentage-- among individuals that we can attribute to genes. So I'll write "due to genes." And so I know that even at this point, that definition is probably not very clear. And we're going to spend the entirety of this video just trying to clarify this concept. And so assume that we're talking about intelligence. And we say that heritability of intelligence is 50%, so IQ. And we've said that the heritability is 50%. And so what we're not saying is that the intelligence is 50% genetic. That's what we're not saying. Really, what we are saying is that the difference in intelligence can be accounted for or is 50% attributable to the genetic differences. So we're talking about the differences in these traits. Because really to ask how much of our intelligence is mandated by our genome as opposed to instilled in us by our environment is completely inappropriate. And that's what we learned in that tea and hot water example. But what can be answered and what those twin and adoption studies started to show us is the percentage of the different, or variance, right here that's attributable to the differences in genes. So I know I've said that several times. And let me move to an example. One of my favorite examples of heritability actually comes from Mark Twain, the famous author and comedian. And he said that suppose you raised four boys in barrels until they were 12 or so. So I've drawn some barrels here. And we've got four boys inside of them. And we'll feed them through a hole so that they get the same diet and such. And we'll remove all of their waste products on the same schedule. And their environments will be 100% controlled, almost sadistically controlled, but 100% controlled. By the age of 12, their IQs would probably be pretty low compared to the population around them. But the kicker is that they're probably not all going to be the same, right? So the differences in their intelligence couldn't be attributed at all to the environment because we've absolutely controlled their environment. And we would say that their IQ differences were nearly 100% heritable because their environments were nearly 100% the same. So h2 is just short for heritability. And we give it a lower case because we're talking about one specific trait, in this case intelligence as opposed to many traits. But the heritability would be close to 100%. And so that was Mark Twain's example of heritability. But alternatively, you could say that maybe you have four identical quadruplets with exactly the same genome. And you raise them in crazy, different environments. And so maybe one is raised in the rain forest as a tribesman. And then maybe one is raised in the desert as a nomad. And then maybe one is raised in a North American family as maybe an industrial worker or at least in an industrial kind of blue-collar family. And then maybe one we'll just say is raised in outer space. He's raised up here near the Moon, maybe on the International Space Station. But the heritability of the intelligence of these four boys would be quite a bit lower because now their environments are counting for a bigger percentage of the differences in their intelligence. And I've said here that the heritability is actually 0% because we've established that they're identical quadruplets. So genetically they're the same. And their environments are completely different. So we would say that all of the differences in their intelligence must be attributed to their environments. So maybe a nice, oversimplified way to think about this idea is that as the environment becomes more and more controlled, like in the example of the boys in the barrels, differences in behavioral traits are more closely tied to heredity. And the heritability of that trait is therefore higher. And then maybe another thing that might increase heritability would be increased genetic variation that leads to different phenotypes. So if there was more genetic variation in these boys, say maybe if they were fraternal quadruplets instead of identical quadruplets and they had different genotypes and more genetic variation leading to different phenotypes or just the expression of their traits, those new variations would be more related to their genes. So in this case, again, the heritability would increase. And so heritability is either increasing because genetics are contributing more to the genes or because nongenetic factors, like the environment, are contributing less. But what matters is that we're talking about the relative contribution of genes to the variation in behavior or traits. And so you might have just caught on that heritability then becomes necessarily dependent on the population that's studied. And so think about one last time, the population of boys inside the barrels. Their heritability is much higher-- or the heritability of their IQ rather is much higher than it would have been if we had instead studied the quadruplets. So one last time, heritability of a trait is the extent to which variation can be attributed to genes. And it's very, very dependent on the populations and the environments that we study. But hopefully, that just gives us a quick context for what we mean when we use this word in the future.