Main content
MCAT
Course: MCAT > Unit 11
Lesson 3: Behavior and geneticsAdaptive value of behavioral traits
Created by Ryan Scott Patton.
Want to join the conversation?
- Under Learned behaviors: permutable, adaptable, and progressive all sound like similar things so can't they be grouped together into one category like "modifiable"?(5 votes)
- The differences are subtle, but Ryan meant these as a contrast to the points stated for Innate behaviours, thats why.(1 vote)
- the Knee jerk patellar reflex example that he talks about nearactually doesn't require brain stimulation according to previous videos so I don't believe the illustrations to correctly portray the concept of behavioral reflexes. 5:53(0 votes)
- its stated the brain represents the central nervous system, so its still correct.(7 votes)
- You make it seem like the barrel guys are real lol(3 votes)
- So a F.A.P is really just a central pattern generator?(1 vote)
- Wow but can you explain it fully and clearly?(1 vote)
- What would walking be? It both is progressive and intrinsic.(1 vote)
- I suspect walking falls within the complex behavior spectrum (See last 45 seconds of video). As child development progresses there are clearly innate aspects to the walking movement. However, it is also something we learn from observation and practice, progressing throughout our childhood.(1 vote)
- What would walking be? It both is progressive and intrinsic.(1 vote)
- I think gait is a learned behavior while the ability to stand upright and move one's legs at all is intrinsic. Children who grow up in the wild without human guardians after whom to model their walking still develop this skill.(1 vote)
- So what's the difference between inherited and intrinsic behavior? The boys in the barrels who still know how to pee because their knowledge was derived from their genes (inherited)(1 vote)
- Inherited behaviours are intrinsic behaviours.(1 vote)
- Would crying be an innate behavior?(1 vote)
- Is there an example of a fixed action pathway for humans?(1 vote)
- Yawning?? Its really difficult to distinguish between reflexes and FAP(1 vote)
Video transcript
OK. So far we've spent a
lot of our time talking about the origins of behavior. And so we've talked about the
genetic component of behavior. And we've talked about the
environmental component of behavior. And we've talked about
how you can tease apart both of those influences
and try to come up with some statistical models
for their relative influence. But today I want
to push past that. And I want to talk about
the function of behavior. Because behavior,
really, at a high level, is a homeostatic mechanism. And so when I say "homeostasis,"
what I'm referring to is the maintenance of a
constant internal condition. And so we're
defining behavior as the coordinated internal
and external response of an organism or
groups of organisms to their environments. So really, behavior
is functioning in the big picture in
a homeostatic sense in the realm of adaptation. So the field that's concerned
with this functional study of behavior,
especially the study of animal behavior in a natural
environment, is ethology. So ethology. And really, ethology
centers on the observations of these animal behaviors. So we call these observable
behaviors overt behaviors. And I want to clarify here,
because sometimes "overt" is used to describe
something that's like hyper obvious or
extremely exaggerated. And I'm just using the
word to mean observable. So simply observable. But behavior gets further
broken down from here, so further broken down past
overt into, generally, three psychological categories. So we have innate behavior. We've got learned behavior. And we've got complex behavior. And we're going to see how
these categories contribute to the adaptive value
of behavioral traits. So first up, we have
innate behavior. And when I say
"innate behavior," I'm talking about behavior
that's genetically programmed. So that's a DNA, because this
is genetically programmed. And generally speaking,
innate behaviors are going to share
some characteristics. And to save a
little bit of time, mostly so you don't have
to watch me write all day, I've gone ahead and
pre-written out this list. But we're going to go
through this list of shared characteristics of
innate behaviors. So first up we have inherited. And what I'm saying when
I say "inherited" is that innate behaviors
are encoded by DNA. And they're going to be passed
to offspring, so inherited. And then next on the
list we have intrinsic. And "intrinsic" means that
these innate behaviors are present in an
animal even if it were to be raised in isolation. So you can think
back to that example that we used in a previous
video of the barrel boys. And we said that
these boys were raised in complete isolation
in a barrel, but they still had
some behavioral traits that were specific to humans. They still breathed like humans. And they still urinated and
they pooped like humans. And we would say that
those behavioral traits are intrinsic. And they're innate. And so further down the list,
innate behavioral traits are stereotypic. And what that means
is that they're performed the same
way each time. So they're stereotypic. And innate behaviors
are also inflexible. And that means that they're
not modified by experience. Again, these are innate,
inside you, not modified by experience. So they're inflexible. And lastly, we say that innate
behaviors are consummate. And what I mean
by "consummate" is that they're fully
developed right away. So at their very
first performance of these behavioral traits,
they're fully developed. These aren't influenced
at all by experience. So because these innate
behavioral traits are encoded by DNA, they're
subject to genetic change through mutations
and recombination and natural selection, just like
any physical trait would be. And so as a particular example,
my wife is currently pregnant. And like many pregnant
women in the first couple months of pregnancy, she
experiences a lot of nausea. So here's my wife. And I will try not
to let her know that I'm drawing her
like this figure. But this is her. And she's pregnant. And she's experiencing
a lot of nausea. And actually, a study
in 2004 suggested this nausea encourages women
to avoid bitter and strongly flavored and novel food, and
that this innate trait might have had an adaptive role in
the past in natural selection by helping women to avoid
toxic foods in the most critical period of
prenatal development. But in general, these
innate behaviors are thought of as programmed. And they usually fit into
one of three categories, the first being reflexes. And reflexes are really the
most basic innate behavior. And they involve a reflex arc. But what you see is a
preprogrammed, automatic, near instantaneous response
to a stimuli involving a sensory nerve
and a motor nerve. So I'll draw a
brain to represent the central nervous
system and the spine. And what you have,
essentially, is a sensory nerve responding to some
stimulus and a motor nerve effecting a preprogrammed, near
instantaneous response to that. And this is just
a looped response that doesn't require
even any thinking. And a great kind
of classic example is the knee-jerk reflex. And when you go to
the doctor office, he may hit your knee
with a little hammer. And he's eliciting
your knee-jerk reflex. But reflexes are one
of the categories of these preprogrammed,
innate behavioral traits. So the next category would
be orientation behaviors. And these are the behaviors that
are involved in regulating us spatially in our environment. And again, these
are preprogrammed. And so these are
automatic responses to the stimulus that
come packaged in our DNA. And one example of
this would be kinesis. And kinesis involves our change
in speed or the change of rate in turning. So change in speed
would be orthokinesis. And a change of rate in
turning would be klinokinesis. But this is in
response to a stimulus. So you can imagine tripping
on a sidewalk and your body naturally changing the speed
of your fall or momentum. And that would be kinesis,
an example of kinesis. And the next one would be taxis. And what taxis is, is a movement
towards-- so a movement towards would be positive taxis--
or a movement away from, and that would be
negative taxis. But movement towards or
away from a stimulus. And so an example of
this that you're probably actually quite familiar with
would be insects and light. So let me draw a light bulb. And if I draw a
little moth, you've seen what happens
to them near light. They are just automatically
attracted to that light source, sometimes to their demise
if it's a bug zapper. But this is an
example of phototaxis. They are moving automatically
towards this stimulus. And that is an innate behavior. And then lastly, we
have this category of innate behavioral traits
called fixed action patterns. So I'll abbreviate that as FAP. And so a fixed
action pattern would be a sequence of
coordinated movements that are performed
without interruption. So very similar to a
reflex, but this time involving coordination
of multiple movements. And an example of this
would be a praying mantis. So this is my praying mantis. And any prey-sized movement that
a praying mantis experiences within striking distance
initiates a strike. And once that
strike is initiated, it involves multiple movements. So it's both of its
arms and its back. But once initiated, it can't be
changed or redirected at all. And so that's a classic example
of a fixed action pattern. And is that third category of
these innate behavioral traits. And so we have these
innate behavioral traits that are functioning
towards our adaptation so that we can maintain
our homeostasis. But also we have learned
behavioral traits. And so learned behavioral traits
are those persistent changes in our behavior that
result from experiences. And so much like innate
behavioral traits, learned behavioral traits have
some kind of general trends to them. And I've written
them down as well to save a little bit of time,
but we'll go through them. And the first characteristic
of learned behavioral traits are that they are non-inherited. So where innate
traits were inherited and they were programmed into
DNA and passed to offspring, these are non-inherited
So they're acquired only through observation
or experience. And then below this,
we have extrinsic. Again, what "extrinsic"
means is that these are absent in animals raised
in isolation from others. So maybe an example of this,
in that barrel boy example that we've been using,
would be social skills. Social skills are
a behavioral trait that would be absent
in these boys that were raised in isolation
inside these barrels. So that's an extrinsic,
learned behavioral trait. And then moving down the
list, we have a permutable. And so "permutable"
means that this is a pattern or a sequence
that can change over time. So you can think of
this as changeable. But then we have adaptable. And I've drawn a little
asterisk here with adaptable, because I don't mean to say that
innate traits don't contribute towards our ultimate adaptation. But this is talking
specifically about the trait. So this actual
trait is adaptable. So it's capable
of being modified. And it's suited to
changing conditions. But that's adaptable. And then last but
not least, learned behavioral traits,
generally speaking, are progressive, which
means that they're subject to improvement or
refinement through practice. But there's going to be
an entire playlist that focuses on the adaptive value of
behavioral change in learning. And that would be a
great place to start exploring the concepts
of habituation, classic versus
operant conditioning, and latent learning,
and imprinting. But it's important
to briefly mention that learning plays a big
role in the adaptive value of behavior. So again, behavior
at a bird's eye view focuses on homeostasis,
right here, and has primarily
an adaptive role. And then the last thing I
want to do in this video is one more time tie
in the relationship between genes and
environment in adaptation. And that fits nicely
into the last category of overt behavior, which
is complex behavior. So like just about everything in
psychology that we've studied, although we introduced the
ideas in absolute terms, so we introduced innate
and we introduced learned, ultimately we tend to
reveal that most ideas fit into a spectrum. And so really, most
behavior is actually somewhere between
innate and learned. And as an example,
let me draw an insect. The ability of an insect to
fly, it starts off as innate. All these insects, they're
born with their wings. And they have this
innate ability to fly. But eventually,
through learning, the insect becomes more
efficient in their abilities of flight. And so this flight
trait, its adaptive role is ultimately
further accentuated. So it starts off
as innate, but it's accentuated through learning. And this would be
a complex behavior. But we've come from studying
the relative role of genes and environment in
forming behaviors. And now we can see
that same relative role of genes and environment
in contributing ultimately to the adaptive role
of these behaviors.