Main content
Secure and insecure attachment
Created by Brooke Miller.
Want to join the conversation?
- You mentioned there is a correlation between the types and of attachment and parenting, but how do we know these aren't just genetic differences? How can we sort out which of the characteristics, both in child and parent behavior, are caused by genetics and which are caused by parenting or other environmental influences?(21 votes)
- That's a very good question! Yes, twin studies have been tried, and, moreover, observing babies shortly after birth when they have had little time to react to the caregiver has given some important clues about other factors than the caregiver. The video seems to indicate that attachment styles is almost all down to the caregiver. Not everyone would agree with that. For instance, it is not possible to conclude which came first: the child's former reaction reaction may have caused a change in the parenting behaviour. We are all geared to respond less if there is little reciprocality.
This reviewed article is in full text and also mentions heritable traits and twin studies. http://www.capmh.com/content/3/1/25
"Several studies have found that newborn behavioural measures are related to later secure and insecure attachment classifications [66-70]. These studies are particularly interesting because neonatal measures reflect minimal social experience."
It is likely that parental care styles do affect attachment significantly, but concluding that if a child does not have a secure attachment does not mean that the caregiver is worse than average at caring for her/his child. In fact, the caregiver can be doing all the right things, and the child still show insecure relationships, or the opposite can be true. Genetic factors and traumatic experiences outside the caregiver's control may cause insecure relationships too. Just think of a child that is hospitalized, and who for various causes cannot be attended by a parent. Or parents who for some reason must let others care for their child for a length of time. Perhaps the parent are hospitalized for a few days: young children may avoid their parents after being cared for by someone else for just a few days, even if their attachment style was regarded as secure before the separation, and afterwards it may take some time to regain the confidence. Adopted children often need their adoptive parents to be around and supportive much more that another child of that age to form a secure relationship with the new parents, even when they were well loved and cared for both before and after their adoption.(6 votes)
- How would a child who had a secure attachment with one parent, and an insecure attachment with the other develop? Would they be somewhere in-between the two categories later in life, or would they tend to gravitate to one or the other?(5 votes)
- This is an interesting thought. I personally think it would be hard to decide. An child's bond with their mother is usually very different from the bond it has with its father. It would also be interesting to look at what kind of attachment the children in this particular study had with their fathers and compare categories to see if there were similarities/differences and how these facts affect the results.(1 vote)
- Could an individual develop insecure attachment during adolescence due to bad experiences with relationships and family members? My behavior is similar to the behavior of someone insecurely attached, but the situations that seemed to have most affected me were in adolescence and during my 20's.(4 votes)
- of course, remember the narrator specifically said only some of the experiments were from childhood attachment studies. at NIH, you can find several more studies on whatever you desire. here's one on Adolescent-parent attachment: Bonds that support healthy development
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2724162/(1 vote)
- Are there any well-known studies on insecure attachment patterns (particularly anxious-ambivalent attachment) and development of pathological traits such as Borderline Personality Disorder? Interested to know. Thanks!(2 votes)
- So much labeling in this video its hard to keep up!(0 votes)
- no one's family is perfect. we just have to learn to deal with problems as they come up.(0 votes)
Video transcript
- [Voiceover] Really young
babies are happy to be passed around to just about everyone, however, something changes
around eight months, and stranger anxiety starts to set in. When this happens, not only is the child uncomfortable around strangers, but even around other people
that they've met before. So, you wind up with
these awkward situtations where the baby doesn't
want to be held by grandma. While this is how most babies react, not all children react the same way. Some definitely seem to have a strong bond with their caregiver, and some kids don't display
stranger anxiety at all. In order to study this behavior and to learn why some
children might deviate from the normal pattern, Mary Ainsworth created
what she refereed to us as the strange situation. Here's how it would work. A mother pictured here in orange, and a child pictured here in yellow, would enter a room in a place
that they hadn't been before, in this case a psychology lab. In the room was a stranger, who was actually part of the experiment, and they're pictured here in blue. For the first part of the experiment, the mother would just sit in a chair, and let their child explore the room. As you note here, that I'm saying mother and not parent or caregiver, because mother-child interactions are what the research
has primarily focused on. This isn't to say that a
child can't form a strong bond with another parent or
with a different caregiver, but here we're just
going to be talking about mother-child relationships. So, we have a mother,
and we have a stranger, and both of them are sitting
in the room with the (childs), but neither is interacting
with the (childs). At some point during the experiment, the mother would get up and leave room, and she would try to do this without calling too much
attention to herself. So, she wouldn't walk over
to her child and say goodbye, instead she would simply
get up quietly and leave. So, the baby is left
alone with a stranger. After a certain amount of time, the mother returns to the room. So, we end the experiment
exactly as we started it, with both the mother and the stranger in the room with the child. Okay, so, that's kind
of a weird situation. What exactly were the
researchers looking for when they set this up? Well, for the first
part of the experiment, they wanted to see
whether or not the child would explore the space
while the mother was present. So, would they get up and walk
and play with all the toys, or would they more reserved
and cling to the mother? The second thing that they're looking at is how the child responds
when the mother leaves. Does the child start
crying when they realize that the mother is gone,
or do they keep on playing? Lastly, they were looking
at how the child reacted to the mother after the
mother returned to the room. So, were they happy about her return, were they sad upon here return, or did they just ignore her all together? After looking at the data, researches found that
they could split children into two main groups. Those with a secure attachment, and those with an insecure attachment. A majority of kids,
about 60 percent of them, demonstrated what was referred
to as a secure attachment. This meant a couple of different things. First of all, the child felt comfortable to explore the room. Maybe, they stayed next to their mother for the first couple of minutes
that they were in the room, but eventually the child
felt comfortable enough to move around and explore. There we times while the child was playing that they might look to the mother, and they might walk back to them. In general, these
children felt comfortable to explore the room on their own. When the parent left, it
was a different story. The children became really upset. They became really distressed
when they noticed her absence. However, that distress tended to go away once the mother returned. When she returned, the children
would typically go to her. They really wanted contact with her. Insecurely attached children showed some different behaviors. When these children were
first brought into the room, they tended to cling to their mother just like the securely
attached children did. However, unlike the
securely attached children, the insecurely attached children tended to stay with their mother. They tended not to explore the room, and just like the securely
attached children, when the mother left the
children became really upset. However, unlike the
securely attached children, that distress didn't really
end once the mother returned. They were upset when she went away, but they weren't soothed by her presence. Other insecurely attached children, even showed avoiding to behaviors. Meaning that they weren't actually upset when the mother left the room, and they were kind of indifferent
to her when she returned. So, now that we know
these two different styles of attachment exists, the next questions is why. What might be responsible
for these differences? What might cause some infants
to be securely attached, while other infants are
insecurely attached? So, when researches started to look deeper into why kids develop
different kinds of attachment, they found that parenting
style had a lot to do with it. Mothers who were sensitive and
responsive to their infants, tended to have children who
exhibited a secure attachment. Mothers who came across as insensitive or unresponsive to their children's needs, tended to form insecure attachments. Which, I'll represent here
by putting a lot of space between the mother and the child. That's not to say that the parents of insecurely attached children we behaving in any way
that would be considered to be really inappropriate. There really isn't any child abuse or serious neglect going on here. These mothers would
attend to their children much like their securely attached peers, but they tended to ignore
them at other times. This is something that you
can probably see for yourself, if you go out and observe some
parent-child interactions. One thing you could look for is how often is the parent
looking at their phone, when their child is trying
to get their attention. It's not to say that
checking your phone is bad or will damage your children in some way. The only questions here seems to be, how long were they
checking their phone for? Did they continue to look at it, even as their child is
trying to their attention? Another question that you might have is whether or not any of
this actually matters? Does it really make that
much of a difference whether or not a child is upset, when a mother enters or leaves a room? Do this really have any long-term effects on a person after childhood? Interestingly enough, the
answer seems to be yes. Some research seems to indicate that a early attachment
style forms the basis of our adult relationships later in life. Specifically, as it relates to our comfort with affection in intimacy. So, individuals who are
securely attached as children, tend to be securely attached in their adult relationships as well. They feel secure in their partners love, and they fell that they can trust them. They really gain a lot of
comfort and security from that. Individuals who are insecurely
attached as children, tend to be insecure and anxious about their relationships when they're adults. They might try to avoid being too attached to any one person. Even more interesting than this, is the fact that our
attachment style as infants, seems to effect the relationships that we have with our own children. So, individuals who
had a secure attachment with their parents, tend to have a secure attachment
with their kids as well. Individuals who had an insecure attachment with their parents, tend to also have children
who are insecurely attached. Take a moment to think about
the implications of this, because this research seems to indicate, that how comfortable we
feel with our parents in our first year of life, continues to effect us all
the way through adulthood.