If you're seeing this message, it means we're having trouble loading external resources on our website.

If you're behind a web filter, please make sure that the domains *.kastatic.org and *.kasandbox.org are unblocked.

Main content

Proprioception and kinesthesia

Proprioception, our sense of body position, comes from tiny sensors in our muscles that signal to our brain. Kinesthesia, on the other hand, is about body movement and learning how to move to accomplish tasks. While both involve body awareness, proprioception includes balance, while kinesthesia does not. Created by Ronald Sahyouni.

Want to join the conversation?

  • blobby green style avatar for user Nelly Bablumian
    I'm confused why when a muscle contracts, it stretches..? Wouldn't it get thicker and shorter?
    (23 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • leaf blue style avatar for user Gage J.
    So, both proprioception and kinesthesia rely on spindles in the muscles to determine where the body is in space, balance, and movement?
    (5 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • leaf yellow style avatar for user Taylor Logan
      Proprioception is reliant on specialized proprioceptive neurons found in muscles and joints, yes - and this helps the body determine where each body part is located and is moving relative to the others.

      However, they should not have used the term 'kinesthesia' because it is not really a medical term and is not used with a consistent definition. A sense of balance and movement of the whole body comes from the vestibular system, which Khan Academy should certainly have a video on. The combination of the two - proprioceptors and the vestibular system - allow the body to sense acceleration and body movement, and thus one's spatial orientation.
      (18 votes)
  • aqualine ultimate style avatar for user fiahmed92
    Can kinesthesia be thought of muscle memory? Around , Ron states that the body is able to detect movements and undergo those movements to successfully complete a task.
    (6 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • aqualine ultimate style avatar for user JLD
      Imagine that you put a blindfold on.

      Ah! You can’t see where your hands are now!

      Yet, you would still likely be able to tell me where your hands are (eg, your left hand may be resting at your side). This would be referred to as proprioception (proprio- meaning “one's own” and -ception meaning perception, thus together proprioception would mean perception of one’s own body). Note how I said resting at your side, this implies that your left hand is not moving.

      Now imagine that I ask you to touch your nose with your left pointer finger (blindfold still on). To avoid smacking yourself in the face, you may slowly move your left pointer finger towards your nose. But wait, how do you know how fast your hand is moving without using your vision? The answer is kinesthesia, the sense of movement and speed (kine- meaning movement and -esthesia meaning feeling, sensation, or perception, thus together kinesthesia would mean perception of movement). In other words, you can sense that you are slowly moving your left finger towards your nose and thus will not have to worry about smacking yourself in the face.

      If you successfully touched your nose while blindfolded, I must ask: how did you avoid poking yourself in the eye? How did you know that it was your left hand and not your right? How did you know that it was your pointer finger and not your ring finger? The answer to these questions is proprioception. You were aware of where your nose was, and you knew where your left pointer finger was, and then you were able to guide the finger to the nose using proprioception and with the correct speed to not hurt yourself using kinesthesia.
      (21 votes)
  • leafers seed style avatar for user t r
    At about , Ron talks about proprioception being more "cognitive", by which he seems to mean subconscious, unthinking, "under the surface". I thought that "cognitive" meant the opposite: awareness, conscious thought, up on the surface. Was I wrong? Can anyone help resolve the meaning(s) of "cognitive" here?
    Thanks.
    (7 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • leaf green style avatar for user Dawn Horan
    Do you have a lesson on the vestibular system? I can't seem to locate any information.

    Kind regards
    Dawn
    (5 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • blobby green style avatar for user Akash Verma
    "Kinesthetic sense is also called proprioception" this is what it says in my Kaplan MCAT review book. But in the video you say they aren't the same. Which one is right??
    (4 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • aqualine ultimate style avatar for user JLD
      Imagine that you put a blindfold on.

      Ah! You can’t see where your hands are now!

      Yet, you would still likely be able to tell me where your hands are (eg, your left hand may be resting at your side). This would be referred to as proprioception (proprio- meaning “one's own” and -ception meaning perception, thus together proprioception would mean perception of one’s own body). Note how I said resting at your side, this implies that your left hand is not moving.

      Now imagine that I ask you to touch your nose with your left pointer finger (blindfold still on). To avoid smacking yourself in the face, you may slowly move your left pointer finger towards your nose. But wait, how do you know how fast your hand is moving without using your vision? The answer is kinesthesia, the sense of movement and speed (kine- meaning movement and -esthesia meaning feeling, sensation, or perception, thus together kinesthesia would mean perception of movement). In other words, you can sense that you are slowly moving your left finger towards your nose and thus will not have to worry about smacking yourself in the face.

      If you successfully touched your nose while blindfolded, I must ask: how did you avoid poking yourself in the eye? How did you know that it was your left hand and not your right? How did you know that it was your pointer finger and not your ring finger? The answer to these questions is proprioception. You were aware of where your nose was, and you knew where your left pointer finger was, and then you were able to guide the finger to the nose using proprioception and with the correct speed to not hurt yourself using kinesthesia.
      (3 votes)
  • purple pi purple style avatar for user coloremotion
    It is not clear to me to find the common ground for both proprioception and kinesthesia.
    (4 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • aqualine ultimate style avatar for user JLD
      Imagine that you put a blindfold on.

      Ah! You can’t see where your hands are now!

      Yet, you would still likely be able to tell me where your hands are (eg, your left hand may be resting at your side). This would be referred to as proprioception (proprio- meaning “one's own” and -ception meaning perception, thus together proprioception would mean perception of one’s own body). Note how I said resting at your side, this implies that your left hand is not moving.

      Now imagine that I ask you to touch your nose with your left pointer finger (blindfold still on). To avoid smacking yourself in the face, you may slowly move your left pointer finger towards your nose. But wait, how do you know how fast your hand is moving without using your vision? The answer is kinesthesia, the sense of movement and speed (kine- meaning movement and -esthesia meaning feeling, sensation, or perception, thus together kinesthesia would mean perception of movement). In other words, you can sense that you are slowly moving your left finger towards your nose and thus will not have to worry about smacking yourself in the face.

      If you successfully touched your nose while blindfolded, I must ask: how did you avoid poking yourself in the eye? How did you know that it was your left hand and not your right? How did you know that it was your pointer finger and not your ring finger? The answer to these questions is proprioception. You were aware of where your nose was, and you knew where your left pointer finger was, and then you were able to guide the finger to the nose using proprioception and with the correct speed to not hurt yourself using kinesthesia.
      (2 votes)
  • female robot grace style avatar for user OpenMinded737
    the mcat requires knowledge of "Kinesthetic sense", but this video doesn't make distinction of it between proprioception and kinesthia. Can I assume its closer to kinesthia as a definition.
    (3 votes)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
    • aqualine ultimate style avatar for user JLD
      Imagine that you put a blindfold on.

      Ah! You can’t see where your hands are now!

      Yet, you would still likely be able to tell me where your hands are (eg, your left hand may be resting at your side). This would be referred to as proprioception (proprio- meaning “one's own” and -ception meaning perception, thus together proprioception would mean perception of one’s own body). Note how I said resting at your side, this implies that your left hand is not moving.

      Now imagine that I ask you to touch your nose with your left pointer finger (blindfold still on). To avoid smacking yourself in the face, you may slowly move your left pointer finger towards your nose. But wait, how do you know how fast your hand is moving without using your vision? The answer is kinesthesia, the sense of movement and speed (kine- meaning movement and -esthesia meaning feeling, sensation, or perception, thus together kinesthesia would mean perception of movement). In other words, you can sense that you are slowly moving your left finger towards your nose and thus will not have to worry about smacking yourself in the face.

      If you successfully touched your nose while blindfolded, I must ask: how did you avoid poking yourself in the eye? How did you know that it was your left hand and not your right? How did you know that it was your pointer finger and not your ring finger? The answer to these questions is proprioception. You were aware of where your nose was, and you knew where your left pointer finger was, and then you were able to guide the finger to the nose using proprioception and with the correct speed to not hurt yourself using kinesthesia.
      (2 votes)
  • blobby green style avatar for user Dale Smith
    Isn't cognitive 'conscious' and not 'subconscious?'
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user
  • female robot amelia style avatar for user ss loves science
    At , Ron says there's a protein inside the sensor which gets stretched. Which protein is he referring to?
    (1 vote)
    Default Khan Academy avatar avatar for user

Video transcript

Imagine if it was pitch black in your room. Would you be able to walk? As long as nothing got in your way, you'd probably be able to walk perfectly fine. But why is this? When walking in a pitch black room, you rely on your sense of balance. You know exactly where you are in space. You know whether or not you're standing straight up or if you're sitting down. And as you're walking, you know if your right foot is in front of your left or if your left foot is in front of your right. So how is it that we know exactly where our body is in space without actually having a look at it? Well, this is known as our sense of propioception. And proprioception is basically defined as our ability to sense exactly where our body is in space. In other words, this is our sense of position. And this sense actually originates from a bunch of tiny little sensors that are located throughout our entire body in almost all of our muscles. So let's imagine that this right here is a muscle in your leg or in your arm. Let's just say it's a muscle in your arm. So there's a tiny little receptor in it, inside of the muscle. And this receptor will actually go up to your spinal cord and then eventually to your brand. And this receptor, if we kind of zoom in on it, we zoom in on this receptor, is sensitive to stretching. So as this muscle contracts, so if I was going to lift something really heavy, the muscle would contract, and it would get thinner, so it would look like this. It would get a bit thinner. And so this sensor, which is known as a spindle-- I'll write that down here. This is a spindle. This sensor can sense that the muscle has been stretched out, and it too will also stretch. So it will go from this conformation to this conformation. It will actually get stretched out. And so we can draw a little spring-like structure inside, which is actually quite similar to what's actually inside the spindle. There's actually a protein inside that gets stretched. When that protein gets stretched, it fires a signal to the brain. This is the basic principle behind proprioception. So we're able to tell exactly how contracted or how relaxed every single muscle is in our entire body, and this allows us to know exactly where our body is in space. There's another word that's commonly used to talk about your body's movements. And this is known as kinesthesia. So let me write that down-- kinesthesia. And so if we talk about proprioception to include your body's position in space as well as your ability to balance, so your sense of balance would be included under proprioception, kinesthesia is talking more about the movement of your body. So one final way to differentiate between the two is that proprioception can be thought of as a cognitive awareness of your body in space. So it's more cognitive. So one way to think about this is that it's a little bit more subconscious. So you're not always thinking about exactly where your body is in space, exactly how you're orientated, if you're walking, or if you're running. Your main concern isn't, oh, I hope-- it might be, but your main concern isn't I hope I'm not going to fall down. Pretty much your sense of balance, your sense of position, are pretty well taken care of by your proprioceptive sense. So it's more cognitive. It's more something that's under the surface. You're not overtly thinking about it. On the other hand, kinesthesia is a little bit more behavioral. And what I mean by that is let's imagine that you're playing golf or you're trying to hit a baseball, constantly you're swinging. Sometimes you miss, sometimes you hit the ball. But every single time you actually swing the bat or swing the golf club, your body is able to detect exactly how it's moving. And so over time, if you learn that, OK, if I move in this certain direction, I'm able to hit the golf ball, or if I move in this direction, I'm able to hit the baseball, then over time, your body is able to detect exactly what that movement is and start to undergo that movement more and more often. So you're able to actually teach yourself exactly how you should move in order to successfully complete whatever task is at hand. So that's just another way to distinguish between the two terms. But just keep in mind that proprioception and kinesthesia are not the same. So they are not the same. So I'll draw a big x here. But they do share a lot in common. And what they do share in common is inferring movement, and position, and where your body is in space. And the big difference is, just to kind of summarize, is that proprioception is concerned with position while kinesthesia is concerned with movement. And proprioception includes your sense of balance, while kinesthesia does not.